Monday, January 13, 2025

How Do Brains Tell Reality From Imagination?

Math and Science News from Quanta Magazine
View this email in your browser
Each week Quanta Magazine explains one of the most important ideas driving modern research. This week, biology staff writer Yasemin Saplakoglu explores how the brain creates images that help guide our reality and imagination.

 

How Do Brains Tell Reality From Imagination?

By YASEMIN SAPLAKOGLU

As I sit at my desk typing up this newsletter, I can see a plant to my left, a water bottle to my right and a gorilla sitting across from me. The plant and bottle are real, but the gorilla is a product of my mind — and I intuitively know that this is true. That's because my brain, like most people's, has the ability to distinguish reality from imagination. If it didn't, or if I had a condition that disrupts this distinction, I'd constantly see gorillas and elephants where they don't exist.  

Imagination is sometimes described as perception in reverse. When we look at an object, electromagnetic waves enter the eyes, where they are translated into neural signals that are then sent to the visual cortex at the back of the brain. This process generates an image: "plant." With imagination, we start with what we want to see, and the brain's memory and semantic centers send signals to the same brain region: "gorilla."

In both cases, the visual cortex is activated. Recalling memories can also activate some of the same regions. Yet the brain can clearly distinguish between imagination, perception and memory in most cases (though it is still possible to get confused). How does it keep everything straight?

By probing the differences between these processes, neuroscientists are untangling how the human brain creates our experience. They're finding that even our perception of reality is in many ways imagined. "Underneath our skull, everything is made up," Lars Muckli, a professor of visual and cognitive neurosciences at the University of Glasgow, told me. "We entirely construct the world in its richness and detail and color and sound and content and excitement. … It is created by our neurons."

What's New and Noteworthy

To distinguish reality and imagination, the brain might have some kind of "reality threshold," according to one theory. Researchers recently tested this by asking people to imagine specific images against a backdrop — and then secretly projected faint outlines of those images there. Participants typically recognized when they saw a real projection versus their imagined one, and  those who rated images as more vivid were also more likely to identify them as real. The study suggested that when processing images, the brain might make a judgment on reality based on signal strength. If the signal is weak, the brain takes it for imagination. If it's strong, the brain deems it real. "The brain has this really careful balancing act that it has to perform," Thomas Naselaris, a neuroscientist at the University of Minnesota, told me. "In some sense it is going to interpret mental imagery as literally as it does visual imagery."

Although recalling memories is a creative and imaginative process, it activates the visual cortex as if we were seeing. "It started to raise the question of whether a memory representation is actually different from a perceptual representation at all," Sam Ling, a neuroscientist at Boston University, told me. A recent study looked to identify how memories and perceptions are constructed differently at the neurobiological level. When we perceive something, visual cues undergo layers of processing in the visual cortex that increase in complexity. Neurons in earlier parts of this process fire more precisely than those that get involved later. In the study, researchers found that during memory recall, neurons fired in a much blurrier way through all the layers. That might explain why our memories aren't often as crisp as what we're seeing in front of us.

Those who are interested in imagination are fascinated by a phenomenon known as aphantasia. Some people — thought to be about 1% to 4% of the general population — don't have a mind's eye: They can't conjure a mental image of a gorilla or visually recall memories. Early studies into their neurobiology suggest that connection differences between brain regions involved in vision, memory and decision-making could explain some cases. However, many people with aphantasia still dream in images, so maybe they "have access to the visual information," Paolo Bartolomeo, a neurologist at the Paris Brain Institute, told me, "but somehow they cannot integrate this information in a subjective experience."

AROUND THE WEB
If someone says, "Don't think of a pink elephant," you will usually think of a pink elephant. Or will you? New research, summarized by the authors at The Conversation, suggests that some people can resist involuntary thoughts.
Scientific American reports how the hippocampus, the part of the brain involved with memory, is also a critical region for imagination.
In a video interview with the Aphantasia Network, Mac Shine, a neuroscientist who studies perception, describes his experience of having aphantasia.
Follow Quanta
Facebook
Twitter
YouTube
Instagram
Simons Foundation

160 5th Avenue, 7th Floor
New York, NY 10010

Copyright © 2025 Quanta Magazine, an editorially independent division of Simons Foundation

Scientist Pankaj

Day in Review: How Many Black Holes Are Hiding? | NASA Awards 2025 Innovative Technology Concept Studies | Space Trivia

An effort to find some of the biggest, most active black holes in the universe provides a better estimate for the ratio of hidden  Mi...